Orwell Award Announcement SusanOhanian.Org Home


Need Children Read "Proficiently" by Grade 3 to Be Sure to Graduate From High School? Some Possible Misinterpretations of Hernandez

Susan Notes:


Stephen Krashen warns againt jumping to quick, easy, and erroneous conclusions from recent Hernandez study.

by Stephen Krashen

Hernandez (2011), based on an analysis of 4000 children studied over ten years, concluded that those who don't read "proficiently" by grade 3 are less likely to graduate high school, and those who live in poverty and do not read "proficiently" by grade 3 are even less likely to graduate high school.

The purpose of this note is to point out possible misinterpretations of these results. Most of these are not Hernandez' conclusions.

Possible misinterpretation 1: Students must reach the "proficient" level by grade three.

There is nothing magic about grade 3.

Hernandez only examined the relationship between reading ability in grade 3 and eventual high school graduation. There is every reason to expect that the same relationship would hold for reading ability at every other grade.

There is also reason to expect, however, that reading ability at any age is not an immutable predictor of dropping out. A great deal of research confirms that students can improve in reading at any age, given the right conditions (Krashen and McQuillan, 2007).

Must all students be "proficient"?

Hernandez accepts the NAEP levels "proficient, basic and below basic," and throughout the paper refers to those who reach the "proficient" level as proficient readers. Gerald Bracey and others have argued, however, that the NAEP levels are arbitrary, and that the cutoff for reaching the "proficient" level has been set much too high, citing an analysis by Rothstein indicating that high-scoring Sweden would only have 33% of its children proficient in reading according to the American NAEP standards (e.g. http://www.schoolsmatter.info/2009/04/bracey-offers-answer-sheet-on-naep.html).

Possible Misinterpretation 2: Poor readers need more traditional reading instruction.

Hernandez assumes that more regular instruction is needed for children of poverty. He notes that children of poverty miss school more than other children do, the problem being that these children are "more likely to lose out on the intensive literacy instruction in the early grades" (p. 11).

In addition, in discussing summer loss in reading, typically experienced by children of poverty, Hernandez recommends that schools need to "develop opportunities for summer learning which are aligned with instruction that occurs during the regular school year" (p. 11).

There is, however, very strong evidence that reading achievement is largely due to extensive, self-selected reading, not to formal reading instruction (Krashen, 2004). Intensive reading instruction focusing on heavy phonics results only in higher performance on tests in which children pronounce words presented in lists; it has no significant effect on tests in which children have to understand what they read (Krashen, 2009).

In addition, research shows that the summer loss in reading is related to lack of access to libraries (and most likely other sources of books) in high-poverty areas (Heys, 1975) and that increased self-selected reading over the summer results in improved reading achievement (Heyns, 1975; Kim, 2003; Shin and Krashen, 2007).

Possible misinterpretation 3: Reading ability and poverty are independent factors.

Hernandez presents data on the impact of reading ability on high school graduation as well as the combined effect of poverty and reading ability.

Based on other studies, it is reasonable to hypothesize that:
(1) Poverty itself has a strong negative influence on reading ability.
(2) Factors other than reading associated with poverty influence high school graduation.
(3) Reading ability influences high school graduation, independent of poverty.

Poverty and reading

A number of studies have confirmed that poverty has a strong negative influence on reading ability (reviewed in Krashen, 2004; 2011). There is an obvious reason for this: Studies (reviewed in Krashen, 2004) consistently show that children living in poverty have little access to books at home, at school, and in their communities. Less access, it has been shown, results in less reading, and less reading results in less literacy development.

Other factors associated with poverty

It is plausible to hypothesize that other factors associated with poverty relate to high school graduation. While those factors mentioned by Hernandez such as health care have been shown to be related to school achievement (Berliner, 2009), the most obvious factor related to high-school completion is poverty itself: Many students have to drop out to work.

Reading ability has an independent influence

Those who read better, regardless of poverty status, will most likely have a better chance of completing high school. Hernandez' data supports this: For those who had experienced poverty as well as those who had not, better readers were more likely to finish high school. [For those who had not experienced poverty, 2% of "proficient" readers dropped out of high school; 14% of "below basic" readers had not. For those who had experienced poverty, 11 of "proficient" readers dropped out, while 31% of "below basic" readers dropped out. Data from Hernandez' Appendix II.]

Hypothesized relationship
NOTE: Unable to reproduce Figure
Figure 1

Reading

Poverty HS completion


Figure 1 hypothesizes that poverty impacts high school completion in more than one way: It influences reading ability, which in turn influences academic success and high school completion (top line of figure 1). In addition, factors associated with poverty (poor health, less health care, see eg Berliner, 2009) influence school performance, and economic pressure results in a higher drop-out rate.

An Additional Concern: The Definition of "Graduation"

The measure of high school completion used was graduation "on time." It does not take into effect those who take significantly longer and those who do the GED. According to federal statistics (US Department of Education, 2010), about 10% of high school graduates take longer than five years (5%) or get GEDs (5%). Not including the full picture biases the analyses against those in poverty, as they are more likely to take longer because of practical constraints.

Discussion

There is no doubt that Hernandez' general findings are correct " both poverty and early reading ability are predictors of the likelihood of completing high school. However:

1. There is nothing crucial about grade three. Hernandez would have probably gotten the same results examining the relationship between reading ability and high school graduation using any other grade.

2. There is nothing crucial about any grade. Students can improve in reading at any age.

3. While the relationships Hernandez found are most likely real, their size may not be accurate: The NAEP levels use to categorize students are problematic, and the measure of high school graduation used overestimates the dropout rate.

4. Poverty itself influences reading ability, and other factors associated with poverty have an impact on the chances of graduating high school independent of reading ability. As Hernandez suggests, we need to deal both with improving reading and we need to at least protect children against the effects of poverty.

We can also increase the chance of high school graduation for high-poverty high school students (and there are more of them all the time) by making course offerings more flexible and by dropping the obsolete and arbitrary notion that everybody "should" graduate high school in four years (Lee and Budzisz, 2010).

No matter how successfully we improve health care and access to books, and even if we succeed in making high school more accessible for high poverty students, economic pressure and the need to earn money will affect dropout rates. Reduction and eventually elimination of poverty is the only sure solution.

5. Improving reading beyond the very beginning stages is a result of doing a great deal of self-selected reading. It is not due to more intensive traditional instruction. To make this possible, our first priority must be to improve access to books among all children, and especially those who live in poverty, and the most obvious first step is to invest in libraries in high-poverty areas.

References
Berliner, D. 2009. Poverty and Potential: Out-of-School Factors and School Success. Boulder and Tempe: Education and the Public Interest Center & Education Policy Research Unit. Retrieved [date] from http://epicpolicy.org/publication/poverty-and-potential

Hernandez, D. 2011. Double jeopardy: How third-grade reading skills and poverty influence high-school graduation. Anne E. Casey Foundation.

Heyns, B. 1975. Summer Learning and the Effect of School. New York: Academic Press.

Kim, J. 2003. Summer reading and the ethnic achievement gap. Journal of Education for Students Placed at Risk 9(2): 169-188.

Krashen, S. 2004. The Power of Reading. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann Publishing Company, and Westview, CONN: Libraries Unlimited. Second Edition.

Krashen, S. 2009. Does Intensive Decoding Instruction Contribute to Reading Comprehension? Knowledge Quest 37 (4): 72-74, 2009

Krashen, S. 2011. Protecting students against the effects of poverty: Libraries. New England Reading Association Journal 46 (2): 17-21.

Krashen, S. and McQuillan, J. 2007. Late intervention. Educational Leadership 65 (2): 68-73.

Shin, F. and Krashen, S. 2007. Summer Reading: Program and Evidence. New York: Allyn and Bacon.

US Department of Education, 2010. Trends in High School Dropout and Completion Rates in the United States: 1972"2008. NCES 2011-012 Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Education.

Lee, E. and Budzisz, M. Saving Students from a Shattered System 2010. Lanham, MD: Rowman and Littlefield

— Stephen Krashen

2011-04-


INDEX OF RESEARCH THAT COUNTS


FAIR USE NOTICE
This site contains copyrighted material the use of which has not always been specifically authorized by the copyright owner. We are making such material available in our efforts to advance understanding of education issues vital to a democracy. We believe this constitutes a 'fair use' of any such copyrighted material as provided for in section 107 of the US Copyright Law. In accordance with Title 17 U.S.C. Section 107, the material on this site is distributed without profit to those who have expressed a prior interest in receiving the included information for research and educational purposes. For more information click here. If you wish to use copyrighted material from this site for purposes of your own that go beyond 'fair use', you must obtain permission from the copyright owner.